Journal of Language, Linguistics and Literature
Articles Information
Journal of Language, Linguistics and Literature, Vol.4, No.1, Mar. 2018, Pub. Date: May 28, 2018
Passive Structure Features in Mandarin-Speaking Children with Specific Language Impairment: Optional Movement
Pages: 8-18 Views: 1810 Downloads: 596
Authors
[01] Tao Zeng, College of Foreign Languages, Hunan University, Changsha, China.
[02] Taoyan Zhu, College of Foreign Languages, Hunan University, Changsha, China.
[03] Xiaoya Li, College of Foreign Languages, Hunan University, Changsha, China.
[04] Rui Zhu, College of Foreign Languages, Hunan University, Changsha, China.
Abstract
Based on the comprehension and production of passives by normal children and children with SLI (Specific Language Impairment), this paper describes the acquisition features of passives by Mandarin-speaking children with SLI. Results show that: 1) The type of passives that both normal Mandarin-speaking children and children with SLI produced most was the patient subject sentence, namely, "Noun (Patient) + Verb + Complement". Children with SLI were not as good as TDA (Typically-Developing Age Matched children) both in the production and comprehension of passives. 2) One of the important linguistic features Mandarin SLI children hold is that syntactic movement in complex sentences is optional, not obligatory.
Keywords
Specific Language Impairment (SLI), Passive Structures, Mandarin-Speaking Children
References
[01] Baldie, B. J. (1976). The acquisition of the passive voice. Journal of Child Language, 3, 331-348.
[02] Bache, C., & Nielson, N. D. (1997). Mastering English: An advanced grammar for non-native and native speakers. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
[03] Bedore, L. & Leonard, L. (2001). Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 905-924.
[04] Bishop, D. V. M. (1979). Comprehension in developmental language disorders. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 21, 225-238.
[05] Bishop, D. V. M. (1992). The underlying nature of specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 3-66.
[06] Borer, H., & Wexler, K. (1987). The maturation of syntax. In T. Roeper & E. Williams (Eds.), Parameter-setting and language acquisition (pp. 123–172). Dordrecht: Reidel.
[07] Borer, H., & Wexler, K. (1992). Bi-unique relations and the maturation of grammatical principles. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 10, 147–189.
[08] Chang, H. W. (1986). Young children’s comprehension of the Chinese passives. In H. Kao & R. Hoosain (Eds.), Linguistics, psychology and the Chinese language (pp. 115–12). Hong Kong: Center of Asian Studies, University of Hong Kong.
[09] Cheung, H. (2008). Grammatical characteristics of Mandarin-speaking children with specific language impairment. In S. P. Law, B. Weeks & A. Wong (Eds.), Language disorders in speakers of Chinese (pp. 33–52). Multilingual Matters.
[10] Chomsky, N. (2001). Derivation by phase. In Kenstowicz, M. & Ken, H. (Eds.), A life in language (pp. 1-52). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
[11] Deen, K. U. (2011). The acquisition of the passive. In J. De Villiers & T. Roeper (Eds.), Handbook of generative approaches to language acquisition (pp. 155–187). Heidelberg London, N. Y.: Springer.
[12] Demuth, K. (1989). Maturation and the acquisition of Sesotho passive. Language, 65, 56–80.
[13] Demuth, K., Moloi, F., & Machobane, M. (2010). 3-Year-olds’ comprehension, production, and generalization of Sesotho passives. Cognition, 115, 238–251.
[14] Ding, S. S., Lv, S. X., Li, R., Sun, D. X., Guan, X. C., Fu, J., et al. (1999). Speeches on contemporary Mandarin grammar. Beijing, China: Commercial Press.
[15] Fletcher, P., Leonard, L. B., Strokes, S. F., & Wong, A. M. (2005). The expression of aspect in Cantonese-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 48, 621–634.
[16] Fox, D., & Grodzinsky, Y. (1998). Children’s passive: A view from the by-phrase. Linguistic Inquiry, 29, 311–332.
[17] Friedmanm, N. & Novogrodsky. R. (2007). Is the movement deficit in syntactic SLI related to traces or to thematic role transfer? Brain and Language, 101, 50-63.
[18] Gordon, P. & Chafetz, J. (1990). Verb-based versus class-based accounts of actionality effects in children’s comprehension of passives. Cognition, 36, 227-254.
[19] Hansson, K., & Nettelbladt, U. (1995). Grammatical characteristics of Swedish children with SLI. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 589–598.
[20] Hirsch, C., & Wexler, K. (2004). Children’s passives and their resulting interpretation. Poster presented at GALANA.
[21] Horgan, D. (1978). The development of the full passive. Journal of Child Language, 5, 65–80.
[22] Huang, C. T. (1982). Logical Relations in Chinese and the Theory of Grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT.
[23] Huang, C. T. (1999). Chinese passives in comparative perspective. The Tsing Hua Journal of Chinese Studies, 29, 423-509.
[24] Huang, C. T. (2007). Thematic structures of verbs in Chinese and their syntactic projections. Linguistic Sciences, 6, 3–21.
[25] Kirby, S. (2010). Passives in first language acquisition: What causes the delay? University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, 16, 109–117.
[26] Leonard, L. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
[27] Leonard, L. (2014). Specific language impairment across languages. Child Development Perspectives, 1, 1-5.
[28] Leonard, L., Eyer, J., Bedore, L., & Grela, B. (1997). Three accounts of the grammatical morpheme difficulties of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 741–752.
[29] Leonard, L., Wong, A., Deevy, P., Stokes, S., & Fletcher, P. (2006). The production of passives by children with specific language impairment: Acquiring English or Cantonese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 267–299.
[30] Li, P. (1991). Zai and Ba constructions in child Mandarin. Center for Research in Language Newsletter, 5, 103-120.
[31] Li, S. (1993). Researches on passive structures in modern Chinese. Beijing, China: Peking University Press.
[32] Li, X. N., Zhou, G. G., & Kong, L. D. (1990). A preliminary observation of application of Ba-construction in Mandarin-speaking Children of two to five years old. Linguistic Research, 4, 43-50.
[33] Li, Y. H. (1990). Order and Constituency in Mandarin Chinese. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
[34] Li, Y. M. (1995). The development of children language. Wuhan, China: Central China Normal University Press.
[35] Lin, B. G., Huang, Y. Z., Huang, G. J., & Xuan, C. H. (2008). Preschool language scale-revised. Taipei: Special Education Center of National Taiwan Normal University.
[36] Liu, H. Y. (2009). PIC and the acquisition of unaccusatives, Ba-constructions and passives in Mandarin-speaking children. Unpublished master’s thesis, Hunan University, Changsha.
[37] Lu, J. M. (2004). Some issues of passive sentences in Mandarin Chinese. Chinese Linguistics, 2, 9–15.
[38] Lukacs, A., Leonard, L., Kas, B. & Pleh, C. (2009). The use of tense and agreement by Hungarian-speaking children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 98-117.
[39] Lv, S. X. (2002). G. Y. Huang (Ed.), Anthology of Lv Shuxiang’s articles. Changchun, China: Northeast Normal University Press.
[40] MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tool for Analyzing Talk (3rd edition). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
[41] Maratsos, C. R., Fox, D. E., Becker, J. A., & Chalkley, M. A. (1985). Semantic restrictions on children’s passives. Cognition, 19, 167–191.
[42] Marshall, C., Marinis, T., & van der Lely, H. (2007). Passive verb morphology: The effect of phonotactics on passive comprehension in typically developing and grammatical-SLI children. Lingua, 117, 1434–1447.
[43] Morehead, D., & Ingram, D. (1973). The development of base syntax in normal and linguistically deviant children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 16, 330-52.
[44] Norbury, C. F., Nash, M., Baird, G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2004). Using a parental checklist to identify diagnostic groups in children with communication impairment: A validation of the children's communication checklist—2. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 39, 345–364.
[45] Oetting, J., & Horohov, J. (1997). Past tense marking by children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 40, 62–74.
[46] Owen, A. J., Hall, J., & Curran, M. (2014). Development of interventions for language impairment: Why universal grammar may be harmful (Commentary on Ambridge, Pine, and Lieven). Language, 90, 131-143.
[47] Pan, H. H., & Han, J. Q. (2005). The syntax of surface unaccusative constructions. Studies in Language and Linguistics, 25, 1–13.
[48] Rice, M., Wexler, K., & Redmond, S. (1999). Grammaticality judgments of an extended optional infinitive grammar: Evidence from English speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 943–961.
[49] Rothweiler, M., & Clahsen, H. (1993). Dissociations in SLI children's inflectional systems: A study of participle inflection and subject-verb-agreement. Scandinavian Journal of Logopedics and Phoniatrics, 18, 169–179.
[50] Sugisaki, K. (1999). Developmental issues in the acquisition of Japanese unaccusatives and passives. In A. Greenhill, H. Littlefield & C. Tano (Eds.). Proceedings of the 23rd annual Boston university conference on language development (pp. 668-683). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
[51] Sybesma, R. (1999). The Mandarin VP. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publisher.
[52] Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., & Zhang, X. (1996). A system for the diagnosis of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1284–1294.
[53] van der Lely, H. (1996). Specifically language impaired and normally developing children: Verbal passive versus adjectival passive sentence interpretation. Lingua, 98, 243–272.
[54] van der Lely, H. (1998). SLI in children: movement, economy and deficits in the computational-syntactic system. Language Acquisition, 7, 161–192.
[55] van der Lely, H., & Battell, J. (2003). Wh-movement in children with grammatical SLI: A test of the RDDR hypothesis. Language, 79, 153–181.
[56] Van der Lely, H., & Ullman, M. (1996). A grammatical specific language impairment in children: An auto- somal dominant inheritance. Brain and Language, 52, 484-504.
[57] Wang, L. (1985). A modern grammar of Chinese. Beijing, China: Commercial Press.
[58] Wang, X. P. (2008). A developmental study of acquisition of narrative skills by children in Taiwan. Unpublished master’s thesis, Fu -Jen Catholic University, Taipei.
[59] Wexler, K. (2004). Theory of phasal development: Perfection in child grammar. In A. Csirmaz, A. Gualmini & A. Nevins (Eds.), MIT working papers in linguistics 48: Plato’s problems: Papers on language acquisition (pp. 59–209). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
[60] Wexler, K., Schaeffer, J., & Bol, G. (2004). Verbal syntax and morphology in typically developing Dutch children and children with SLI: How developmental data can play an important role in morphological theory. Syntax, 7, 148–198.
[61] Wexler, K. (2011). Grammatical impairments in developmental syndromes: An experimental and theoretical overview, Paper presented at the 5th International Conference on Formal Linguistics. Guangzhou, China.
[62] Xu, T., & Yang, X. L. (2008). Children’s acquisition of passives in Chinese. In Otsu, Yukio (Ed.), The Proceedings of the Ninth Tokyo Conference on Psycholinguistics. Tokyo: Hituzi Shobo.
[63] Xu, T. (2010). Are Chinese children’s passive delayed? In Otsu, Yukio (Ed.), The Proceedings of the Eleventh Tokyo Conference on Psycholinguistics. Tokyo: Hituzi Shobo.
[64] Yang, X. L., & Xiao, D. (2008). A case study of acquisition of Ba-construction in Mandarin. Contemporary Linguistics, 3, 200-210.
[65] Yang, Z. (2009). A quantitative analysis of the occurrence ratio of discourse in contemporary Chinese passive constructions. Modern Chinese, 8, 55–58.
[66] Zhang, F. F. (2010). The Study on the Narrative Development of Young Chinese Children with Specific Language Impairment Aged 4-6. Phd. Dissertation. Shanghai: Huadong Normal University.
[67] Zhou, G. G. (1997). The acquisition of syntactic structures in Mandarin Chinese. Hefei, China: Anhui University Press.
[68] Zhou, G. G., Kong, L. D., & Li, X. N. (1992). Passive structures in child language. Applied Linguistics, 1, 38–48.
[69] Zhu, D. X. (1982). Lectures on grammar. Beijing, China: Commercial Press.
[70] Zou, K. (1995). The Syntax of the Chinese BA-constructions and Verb Compounds: A Morpho-Syntactic Analysis. California: University of Southern California.
600 ATLANTIC AVE, BOSTON,
MA 02210, USA
+001-6179630233
AIS is an academia-oriented and non-commercial institute aiming at providing users with a way to quickly and easily get the academic and scientific information.
Copyright © 2014 - American Institute of Science except certain content provided by third parties.